GOODSOUND!GoodSound! "Editorial" Archives

February 1, 2005

 

Understanding Cables and Wine

The value of specialty interconnects, speaker cables, and power cords remains one of the most hotly debated topics among audio enthusiasts. Whereas most people will concede that source components, such as CD players, can sound different from one another, there remains a great deal of skepticism about the differences in a system’s sound that can be achieved by changing cables. On the face of it, such skepticism may seem reasonable, but I want to examine here some reasons for thinking that, in the end, such skepticism is not well founded.

One reason for the skepticism comes from a dogmatic reliance on measurements. The use of measurements in describing audio products goes back many years, and perhaps reached its zenith in the late 1970s and early ’80s, when bigger numbers automatically meant better. By relying solely on measurements, a company could market an amplifier as having a power rating of hundreds of watts. Told that more watts were better than fewer, consumers shopped accordingly. Thus, buying decisions were made based not on how components sounded, but on how they measured. This reliance on numbers continues today at such large retailers as Best Buy and Circuit City. Employees at such stores can tell you how many watts an amplifier produces or how many inputs a receiver has, but few can tell you how a component sounds.

Reliance on measurements, then, makes good marketing sense for electronics manufacturers and retailers; it is much more easy to relay such information to a sales staff than to rely on -- or train -- that staff to be discriminating listeners. The audio marketplace thus has a built-in reliance on measurements. When cables are approached with that mentality, it seems that all cables should be pretty much the same.

The dependence on measurements goes deeper than a simple marketing tool, however. It is part of the 20th-century zeitgeist, which embraced a scientific methodology that relies essentially on quantitative methods for investigating and understanding the world. There is a pervasive belief that if there is a difference between two things, then that difference must be measurable. The leap is then made that if there is no measurable difference, then there is no difference. There are also the common assumptions that what affects a listener’s perception of sound is what is measurable in the lab, and that we have a much better understanding of the sound-producing properties of audio systems and of our own auditory perceptions than we actually do.

However, the failure to measure an audible difference does not indicate that there is no difference. It is possible that we do not understand what needs to be measured, or that there are too many variables for us to make a rigorously controlled study of the differences in sound. But this would still not indicate that there is no difference. What it does indicate is that science remains an insufficient tool for understanding all aspects of the world. In any of these cases, a reliance on measurements is not the only way to evaluate components or cables; we can also rely on our own perceptions.

Another reason for skepticism about cables is the fervor with which some enthusiasts discuss the differences cables can make. I think that, for some audio enthusiasts, cables represent an affordable change that they can make to their systems, and which they have embraced as a hobby unto itself. That is a fine hobby to have, but sometimes when such hobbyists talk about how cables sound, they begin to sound a bit mystical. A cable on its own has no sound -- it needs to be connected to other components to produce sound. When you’re evaluating a cable, you’re actually evaluating the whole signal chain, from source through to the speakers. Changing cables may alter the sound, but to then ascribe a sound to the cable is not correct. What cables contribute to the sound must be considered along with the components they are used with. Cables may sound great with some components, not so great with others. Like just about everything, cables are not intrinsically good or bad, but are better or worse only in relation to the rest of the system in which they are used.

I think being able to perceive differences in a system’s sound with different cables is akin to being able to discern fine wine from merely good wine. If you’re a wine drinker, think back to when you first began to drink. At the beginning, people are often unable to describe what they taste, and aren’t even sure which wines they like and which they don’t. Over time, people develop the ability to discriminate between the wines they enjoy and those they dislike. Those who begin to care seriously about wine go on to learn how to describe their experiences in critical terms so that they can discuss with others not only which wines they like, but what about those wines they like so much.

Consider our friend Chris, who has recently become a wine enthusiast. In the past few months Chris has decided he likes very much the 2002 Veritas Vineyard Shiraz Christa Rolf. When he first tasted it, all he could tell us was that he liked it -- but after reading a lot of Robert Parker and attending weekly wine tastings, Chris can now tell us that the shiraz is "plump" and "spicy," has "hints of licorice and earth," and is "nicely textured." He is now happy to spend $14 for a bottle of this wine and think it a great bargain. Another friend, Randall, has no interest in wine and thinks Chris is making up all of these fancy descriptions. For him, wine is wine, and $14 is twice as much as the cheapest bottle he can get. Randall thinks Chris is out of his mind to spend so much.

I think most of us would assume that Chris has simply become a more discerning wine drinker. All of the properties that Chris can now discern -- such as "hints of licorice" -- were always there, but he lacked the skills to articulate their presence. Randall, however, has no interest in wine, and so has never seen the point in acquiring the skills necessary to describe its tastes in such detail. Science, too, has failed to come up with quantifiable tests for "hints of licorice" or "nicely textured," but most of us do not assume that Chris is therefore making up the differences he describes.

If Chris’s approach to wines is something you think is reasonable, then there’s no reason a similar approach to the different sounds of different cables would not be just as reasonable. Just as Chris needed to train himself to discern subtle differences among wines, a budding audio enthusiast needs to train him- or herself to discern subtle differences in sound.

The only way to know if you like a particular wine or a particular cable is to try it. Even if you understand Chris’s description of his favorite wine, without tasting it yourself, you won’t know if you’ll like it as much as he does. If you’re a wine enthusiast, his description will probably give you a better sense of whether or not you’ll share his taste, but only actually tasting the wine will confirm that suspicion. This is true with cables as well. You may read a description of a cable that makes it seem as if you’d enjoy the difference it would make in the sound of your system, but only an audition of that cable in that system will tell you for sure. And having a nice glass of wine while you listen won’t hurt.

…Eric D. Hetherington


GOODSOUND!All Contents Copyright © 2005
Schneider Publishing Inc., All Rights Reserved.
Any reproduction of content on
this site without permission is strictly forbidden.